Articles

Industry News

Top MOOCs

Binge Learning: What Online Education Can Learn from Netflix

By Henry Kronk
September 22, 2017

As new digital methods of hosting content arise, we change the way we consume it. Whether it’s a MOOC (massive online open course) or a series on Netflix, with greater availability comes the increased ability to consume more, faster. Most people know this as binging. Most people—especially if they only caught on to Game of Thrones during season 4—have spent hours of their free time watching episode after episode.

We generally consider binging a bad thing, but a new study conducted by Wharton professors Eric Bradlow, J. Wesley Hutchinson, and doctoral candidate Tong Lu, suggests that binging content helps us learn better.

Their study examined students taking a class on Coursera, one of the most widely used online education platforms. They received and analyzed data regarding how quickly students in multiple different classes consumed each learning module and compared it to their performance in the class.

Temporal vs. Content Binging

To begin, the researchers identified two different types of binging. The first is much like what someone might do with a Netflix series: they watch episode after episode or proceed through module after module in quick succession. They called this “temporal” binging. The other kind involves focusing on one subject intently. For example, a student might make it all the way through their marketing course before they begin history. They referred to this as “content” binging.

Binging Vs. Non-Binging in E-Learning

After the researchers analyzed student learning patterns, they compared them to their grades. To their surprise, the students who binged did better than those who did not. Bradlow, furthermore, had research he conducted on consumer binging habits with Netflix and Hulu three years earlier. During that study, he found that binge consumers were more likely to become lifelong consumers. He was now able to draw a few conclusions:

“[I]n our case on Coursera,” Bradlow told the Knowledge@Wharton podcast, “people who binge consume marketing and operations are more likely to complete their finance course. They’re more likely to complete their accounting course. Just like we found in our work three years ago, it appears that binge consumption leads to higher customer lifetime value. That was the key takeaway. We’ve just reinforced that and basically replicated that in an online educational content setting.”

There are still a few questions left to be answered. For one, it’s difficult to determine which type of binging leads to better results. Many students participated in both temporal and content forms of binging. Others binged for a certain period before reverting to non-binging tactics. The study, furthermore, asked if binging helped the learning process, not why. That remains quite mysterious.

Sequential vs. Simultaneous Release

What remains clear is that when designing a content-hosting platform—whether it’s an e-learning initiative or a video streaming service—it’s imperative to allow for the practice of binging. Before the Internet era, content was delivered in non-binge-able formats. TV series would release one episode per week. Serialized books were published in sections. This is what’s known as sequential release: small bytes of content that come out periodically.

Online platforms, however, have adopted a new strategy: simultaneous release. This makes all content available immediately. It doesn’t affect people who prefer not to binge, but it allows bingers to consume with abandon.

As Barlow explains, “There’s a known phenomenon in the behavioral literature called goal gradient. People accelerate their consumption of something as they get near the end. As you get near the goal, your gradient increases. In other words, you work harder as you can see the finish line.”

Binge Learning vs. Distributed Practice

Bradlow, Hutchinson, and Lu’s results are surprising because most research indicates that non-intensive and varied studying practices improve knowledge retention. Fast, intensive learning does not stick as well. Studies by countless academics have proven this point.

But here’s the clincher: most of these studies have involved students cramming in a traditional university class, and not people enrolled in MOOCs. University crammers have likely skipped most classes and hope to learn everything in the course of a few late nights. Online students, however, spend roughly the same amount of time as students who spread their studies out. It is likely that much of the previous research does not apply to MOOCs, especially those in which students must go through module by module. In these scenarios, cramming is impossible.

As Bradlow contends, his team’s findings have implications not only for online learning but for businesses and any other application of content-hosting platforms. Their research is far from over and there is little doubt they will refine their results in the future.